Skip to content

Mella Lira, B., Paez, A. (2021) Do drivers dream of walking? An investigation of travel mode dissonance from the perspective of affective values, Journal of Transport and Health, 20:101015

Notifications You must be signed in to change notification settings

paezha/Mode-Dissonance-and-Subjective-Wellbeing

Repository files navigation

Do Drivers Dream of Walking? An Investigation of Travel Mode Dissonance from the Perspective of Affective Values

Beatriz Mella Lira (Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile)
Antonio Paez (McMaster University)

Journal of Transport and Health (2021) Vol. 20:101015, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2021.101015

Abstract

Introduction

Subjective wellbeing is a topic that has attracted considerable attention in the transportation literature in recent years. As a result, there is a burgeoning literature that investigates the impacts of travel on subjective wellbeing, and how wellbeing, in turn, can influence behavior. An important aspect of subjective wellbeing are the affective reactions of people to their experiences.

Objective

The objective of this paper is to analyze the affective reactions of travelers with respect to various modes of transportation. In particular, we are interested in the potential for dissonance between primary mode of travel and the mode(s) of travel identified as evoking various affective reactions.

Materials and Methods

The study is based on data collected from a sample of travelers in the city of Santiago, in Chile. Participants in the study were asked about their usual mode of travel, and then were asked to name their ideal mode(s) of transportation from the perspective of various affective reactions. The reactions we investigate are associated with the values of Freedom, Enjoyment, Happiness, Poverty, Luxury, and Status. Analysis is based on tests of independence and visualization techniques.

Results

The results indicate that users of public transportation experience the most dissonance in terms of affective reactions, and active travelers the least. For those travelers who experience dissonance, active travel is the mode most commonly associated with Freedom, Enjoyment, and Happiness, while public transportation is most commonly associated with Poverty. The automobile, in contrast, is the mode most commonly associated with Luxury and Status.

Keywords

  • Mode of travel
  • Subjective wellbeing
  • Dissonance
  • Affective values
  • Santiago de Chile
  • Survey
  • Data analysis

Introduction

Transportation planning for decades has focused on providing mobility for the private automobile. This is a model of development that was initially introduced in North America as a solution to problems created by rapid urbanization, and that was eventually copied elsewhere (Brown, Morris, and Taylor 2009; Angotti 1996). Despite the initial promise of automotive technology, it is now evident that mobility centered on the private automobile has given rise to a litany of maladies that are in urgent need of correction. This includes environmental concerns (i.e., climate change; Chapman 2007) as well as numerous other social (Boschmann and Kwan 2008; Lucas 2012, 2019), health (Khreis et al. 2016; Milne 2012), and equity issues (Bocarejo and Oviedo 2012; Martens, Golub, and Robinson 2012; Pereira, Schwanen, and Banister 2017).

As the impacts of our societal dependence on the private automobile have become increasingly evident, the transportation agenda has aimed to shift focus to the reduction of car use and towards the creation of mobility polycultures that offer a broader menu of transportation alternatives other than primarily (or even just) the private automobile (Miller 2011; Lavery, Paez, and Kanaroglou 2013). In order to successfully achieve this goal, it is essential not only to provide the services and facilities that support public transportation and active travel, but also to attract new users to these modes of transportation (Ettema et al. 2011). Within this context, it has been argued that sustainable transportation policies require all participants in the transportation system to challenge what Gossling and Cohen (2014) termed transportation taboos: deep-seated ideas concerning the contribution to emissions by individuals, the inequality of market-based approaches, and the social and psychological functions of transportation. With respect to the latter, subjective wellbeing (SWB) is increasingly recognized as a policy goal in and of itself (Diener, Oishi, and Lucas 2015; Dolan and Metcalfe 2012), and also as an intermediate objective to achieve ulterior goals. Accordingly, there is a growing consensus in the field of transportation about the need to move beyond a purely utilitarian focus to understand the affective and wellbeing values of the transportation experience (e.g., Anable and Gatersleben 2005; De Vos et al. 2013; Domarchi, Tudela, and Gonzalez 2008; Gatersleben and Uzzell 2007; Steg 2005). Consideration of factors related to subjective wellbeing, for example, are useful for policy-makers to enhance the experience of travel, to increase satisfaction with travel, and to meet the preferences of travelers (Chatterjee et al. 2019).

A definition of subjective wellbeing is as “[g]ood mental states, including all of the various evaluations, positive and negative, that people make of their lives, and the affective reactions of people to their experiences” (OECD 2013, 10). As memorably put by Steg (2005): is the use of a car a must, or is it a lust? A close alignment, or consonance, between affective reactions and the mode of transportation used to travel can result in greater subjective wellbeing and increase the probability of choosing that mode (i.e., by satisfying the lust); on the other hand, dissonance, namely the lack of concordance between the mode used and the affective reaction to that mode, would be detrimental to subjective wellbeing, especially to those for whom the use of the mode is a must (J. De Vos 2019; Mokhtarian and Pendyala 2018). With these considerations in mind, the present research aims to investigate who experiences affective dissonance with respect to their primary mode of travel. We focus our investigation on reactions from the perspective of feelings of Freedom, Enjoyment, Happiness, Poverty, Luxury, and Status - concepts termed affective values hereafter. Furthermore, it is possible, even if use of a particular mode is a must, that travelers may still lust for something else - in other words, the grass may look greener from the window of a car. For this reason, we also aim to investigate which mode or modes are most commonly identified as ideal from the perspective of the aforementioned affective values.

Our research is based on data collected from a sample of travelers in the city of Santiago in Chile. The analysis sheds light on the affective reactions of people towards their primary mode of travel, and their perceptions towards ‘ideal modes’. This reveals important differences in situations when the ideal mode of a traveler is not the mode they actually use - or in other words, the affective dissonance between actual and ideal modes of travel. More concretely, the results indicate that users of public transportation experience the most dissonance, followed by automobile users, and finally active travelers, who tend to experience the least affective dissonance. For those travelers who experience dissonance, active travel is the mode most commonly associated with Freedom, Enjoyment, and Happiness, whereas public transportation is most commonly associated with Poverty, and the automobile is most commonly associated with Luxury and Status. We also find that there are significant variations in dissonance by age, education, income, and typical commute time. The reactions of travelers towards various transport modes are critical factors that policy-makers need to consider to promote and increase the use of public transport and active modes (Bornioli, Parkhurst, and Morgan 2019; De Vos and Witlox 2017; J. De Vos et al. 2019; Garling, Bamberg, and Friman 2019; Redman et al. 2013).

After these introductory remarks, the paper follows up with a review of the literature on the topics of subjective wellbeing and affective values. Next, we discuss the case study, as well as the methodology and data used for this research. The analysis and results of the study are presented afterwards, before concluding with some discussion and directions for future research and policy.

Please note that this document was prepared using R Markdown and is an example of reproducible research (Brunsdon and Comber 2020). The R Markdown file, along with the data file needed to reproduce the analysis, are available for download here.

Background

A consensus has emerged in recent years regarding the need to complement the traditional utilitarian perspective of transportation by investigating mobility and transport from the lens of their affective functions. The affective value of transportation is important due to its potential to improve or detract from SWB.

One of the primary ways to explore issues of subjective wellbeing in transportation has been the satisfaction that travelers feel towards their everyday mobility experience (e.g., Jakobsson Bergstad, Gamble, Gärling, et al. 2011). As a consequence, there is a wealth of research on satisfaction with the use of different modes of transportation. For example, numerous studies report that car users often have a higher level of satisfaction compared to other transport modes (Jakobsson Bergstad, Gamble, Hagman, et al. 2011; Eriksson, Friman, and Gärling 2013; Redmond and Mokhtarian 2001), although others report highest commute satisfaction for bicycle and train commuters (e.g., Whalen, Páez, and Carrasco 2013; Handy and Thigpen 2019). In a similar way, there are multiple reports that active travel also tends to yield higher levels of satisfaction (Gatersleben and Uzzell 2007; Smith 2017; St-Louis et al. 2014; Paez and Whalen 2010; Whalen, Páez, and Carrasco 2013; Handy and Thigpen 2019), In contrast, public transport users often assess their experience more negatively (Abenoza, Cats, and Susilo 2017; De Vos et al. 2016; Gatersleben and Uzzell 2007; Handy and Thigpen 2019; Paez and Whalen 2010). Multi-modal trips also influence satisfaction levels; for instance, individuals tend to report higher levels of satisfaction with the mode they use – perhaps as a form of post hoc validation of their choices or circumstance (Susilo and Cats 2014).

While travel satisfaction has often been used in the context of daily trips – typically linked to cost-benefit and utilitarian measurements –, the evaluation of subjective wellbeing (SWB) over time has risen as an alternative measure. Ettema et al. (2010, 725) define SWB as the degree to which an individual positively evaluates the overall quality of their lives, where the general life satisfaction encompasses a more extended temporal period – which implies greater temporal stability. This concept has prompted a growing literature that complements and applies SWB in a broader range of satisfaction scales and situations. The definition of other factors such as travel choice mode, attitudes, and external elements of the built environment have been studied for a broader understanding of the changes produced in the SWB (e.g., Handy and Thigpen 2019). As these factors do not necessarily apply to longer-term general life satisfaction, other studies have aimed to determine both the direct and indirect effects on the perception of users (see, e.g., Ye and Titheridge 2017). Other concepts have also emerged, including the Satisfaction with Travel Scale (Ettema et al. 2011), as well as different scales based on people’s perceptions of travel. De Vos et al. (2015), for instance, explore in detail the underlying dimensions of the affective domain of STS on which SWB is based (for more on STS also see Friman et al. 2013).

The literature on SWB has demonstrated a relationship between people’s perceptions and satisfaction with their daily travel (e.g., Smith 2017; St-Louis et al. 2014; Mokhtarian and Pendyala 2018). Scholars have shown that accessibility is among the most studied factors that influences subjective wellbeing (Delbosc 2012), and activities have a direct impact on travel satisfaction (Jakobsson Bergstad, Gamble, Gärling, et al. 2011). Delbosc (2012, 28), for instance, has summarized the most significant influences on psychological wellbeing: poverty and employment, meaningful relationships and health. However, understanding the components that affect these perceptions implies the differentiation between affective (also named as symbolic-affective) and instrumental values (Jakobsson Bergstad, Gamble, Hagman, et al. 2011). Steg et al. (2011) have compared symbolic-affective and instrumental-reasoned motives for car-use, and similar findings are reported in other studies (e.g., Gatersleben and Uzzell 2007; Lois and Lopez-Saez 2009). Previous research also demonstrates how socio-demographic factors affect the levels of SWB, including income (Clark and Oswald 1996; Ferrer-i-Carbonell 2005), education and unemployment (Argyle et al. 1999), age (Diener and Eunkook Suh 1997), and gender (Tesch-Römer, Motel-Klingebiel, and Tomasik 2008). However, further research beyond the determinants of trip satisfaction is needed to understand how these socio-demographic variables connect with the affective reactions to various modes of travel (St-Louis et al. 2014).

The research needs outlined above are well-recognized in the developed world, but there is still a dearth of research in the context of the Global South. Historical inequality in many developing countries has led to strong symbolic attachments to the automobile, in addition to negative connotations for public transport and active travel (Zorrilla, Hodgson, and Jopson 2019). To address this gap in knowledge a body of literature has emerged to investigate the affective aspects of travel behavior in a number of developing countries. A cross-country study in Asia revealed that the affective factors of public transportation and car use are important, and in particular the social orderliness of transit was suggested as a way to make this mode more attractive to users (Van, Choocharukul, and Fujii 2014). In terms of active travel, a study in China found that attitudes that embrace new styles and technologies despite their cost are associated with the intention to continue using shared bicycles (Shao and Liang 2019). The importance of affective factors for policy and planning is further highlighted by research in Colombia that shows the pride users feel when using a bicycle shared system. This affective reaction is above and beyond other positive feelings, such as a sense of belongingness to a civic culture, and the enjoyment and pleasure of active travel itself (Bejarano, Ceballos, and Maya 2017).

Case Study and Data

Context

The case study for the present research is Santiago. Santiago is the capital of Chile, a country with one of the highest levels of socio-economic inequality in the world. A tangible manifestation of the inequalities experienced by many in Chile is the large disparity in the relative cost of transportation, travel time, and distance traveled by different socio-economic segments of the population. To further complicate matters, strong spatial segregation also conditions car ownership and the use of public transportation – the higher the income, the higher the use of the automobile; conversely, the lower the income, the higher the reliance on public transportation. Although the transportation experience is but one of many dimensions of inequality, the experiences in this sector have triggered exceptional discomfort and dissatisfaction among the public. Recent social unrest, triggered by a seemingly minor hike in the fare of public transportation, brought many of these concerns to the forefront of the public conscience in dramatic fashion (e.g., Davies 2019).

Previous research has helped to contextualize everyday mobility in Santiago relative to other Latin American cities (e.g., Avellaneda and Lazo 2011; Rodríguez Vignoli 2008), but much remains to be explored. Measuring instruments and new methods have led to more accurate and precise understandings of the social issues that arise as consequence of transport infrastructure and housing provision (Cox and Hurtubia 2016), and the minimum provision of basic services (Tiznado-Aitken, Muñoz, and Hurtubia 2016). However, the focus on accessibility as a measure of inequalities remains predominant (Tiznado-Aitken, Muñoz, and Hurtubia 2016; Martínez et al. 2018; Niehaus, Galilea, and Hurtubia 2016; Rojas et al. 2016).

Data

The study is based on a survey conducted in the city of Santiago during the months of November and December of 2016, at the end of the Spring and beginning of Summer seasons. The survey collected information on a wide range of transportation and related issues, and the data collection protocol considered a quota-sampling method for gathering the information, considering the socio-demographic information from Pre-Census of 2012. The survey was carried out face-to-face in centers of activity with dense provision of offices, services, and educational centers. An equal representation of both genders and a representation of the proportion of inhabitants per area were chosen as relevant characteristics of the sample. In total, there were (n=451) valid responses, although not every response was complete and some questions have missing values.

The study considers data from 3 out of 8 sections of a longer survey. The relevant sections of the survey concern the individual characteristics of respondents, their feelings and affective responses related to their commute, and aspects describing their regular commute trips. In terms of individual characteristics and their commute, participants were asked socio-demographic information, including age (coded in three classes: younger than 35, older than 35 but younger than 55, and 55 and older); level of education (three classes: Kindergarten to grade 12, or K-12, technical diploma or university graduate, and graduate degree), income (three classes, by tertile), and the typical duration of the respondent’s regular commute (four classes, by duration in minutes). The descriptive statistics of the sample appear in Figure . The sample trends to younger (56% is younger than 34 years old) and well-educated respondents (68% of respondents having technical or professional education), with an almost uniform distribution of income levels. The trend in typical commute time is towards longer commutes – e.g. 55% of the trips are longer than 40 minutes, and from those 56% corresponds to trips over the 60 minutes long.

Descriptive statistics of the sample

In addition, respondents were asked about their primary mode of travel for their regular commute. The modes available were Car; Taxi; Colectivo (a form of shared ride, intermediate in flexibility and capacity between taxi and bus); Motorcycle; Metro; Bus; Bicycle; and Walking. As seen in the top panel of Figure , the three most common modes of travel are Metro, Bus, and Car, followed by Walking and Bicycle. For the analysis, we aggregate these modes into the following categories (bottom panel of Figure ): (26% of total modes – which corresponds to 91% of the total of private motorized modes), Active (9%

  • Walking Bicycle), Public (60% - Metro Bus), and Other motorized modes (6% - Taxi Colectivo Motorcycle).

Frequency of primary mode used for regular commute; top panel: all modes, bottom panel: aggregated modes

Of particular interest for the present study is the following question in Part 3 of the survey:

Q: Please indicate the mode(s) of transport that you relate to the following feelings and concepts

The question was asked for each of the following affective values: Freedom; Enjoyment; Happiness; Poverty; Luxury; and Status. The respondents were not constrained to select only one alternative, but could indicate by means of checkboxes any and all modes that they felt aligned with each affective value. This allows us to analyze the mode dissonance. Dissonance is a concept introduced into the transportation literature by Schwanen and Mokhtarian (2004) based on earlier work by Feldman (1990). Originally used to investigate residential dissonance (Schwanen and Mokhtarian 2004), the concept has since been extended in the travel behavior literature to encompass the mismatch between the choices individuals make, and the alternatives that would enable users to improve their experience. This includes, for example, travel mode dissonance (De Vos 2018).

Based on the primary mode of travel and the questions about affective values, we derived a series of travel mode dissonance variables according to the following rule: [ D_{ij} = \begin{cases} 0 & if \text{ primary mode of traveler }i == \text{mode associated with value } j\ 1 & \text{ otherwise} \end{cases} ] Therefore, if respondent (i) travels primarily by Car, but indicated any other mode(s) in relation to Freedom, the respondent experienced dissonance: [ D_{i,\text{Freedom}} = 1 ]

In these calculations we account for all modes identified by respondents in relation to the affective values. Therefore, to avoid double counting the respondents in our frequency tabulations, we also calculated a sample weight as the inverse of the number of modes selected in response to each affective value. For instance, for respondents who selected two modes in relation to affective value (j), each of these two modes receive a weight of (w_{ij}=1/2); if a respondent selected three modes, then their weights are (w_{ij}=1/3); and so on. In this way we do not treat unfairly those respondents who selected only one mode, and the sum of all weighted responses is equal to the size of the sample (n).

Results

In what follows, the results refer to two related but distinct questions. The first part of the analysis seeks to understand who experiences dissonance, whereas the second part, building off that, aims to explore which modes are more commonly identified as embodying affective values by those travelers who experience dissonance.

Who experiences dissonance?

We begin by profiling the travelers who experience dissonance. The null hypothesis is that there are no systematic differences in terms of who tends to experience affective dissonance with respect to their primary mode of travel. To investigate this hypothesis we create contingency tables to tabulate the frequency of dissonance with respect to each affective value, stratified by the individual attributes of respondents. Table presents the frequency (in percentage) of dissonance. This is the percentage of respondents out of the total in their stratum who selected, for each affective value, a mode or mode(s) of transportation that do not correspond to their primary mode of travel.

As seen in the table, there are five individual attributes that we analyze. Three of these are socio-economic and demographic, namely age, level of education, and income. The other two are transportation related, namely primary mode of travel and commute time. The frequency tables are tested in every case using (\chi^2) tests of independence ((p)-values are reported in the table; lower (p)-values mean that the null hypothesis of independence can be rejected with greater confidence). It is interesting to note that the only category for which all affective values are significant ((p\leq0.05)) is mode of travel.

Age

With respect to age, previous studies have reported that older adults tend to be more satisfied with their travel experience than younger people (Cao and Ettema 2014; De Vos et al. 2016; Ye and Titheridge 2017). In the case, we find that there there are significant differences in dissonance by age with respect to five affective values, namely Freedom, Enjoyment, Happiness, Luxury, and Status. We observe that levels of dissonance tend to be high in general, and in no case less than 60%. For instance, almost 90% of travelers younger than 35 experience travel mode dissonance with respect to Freedom, and more than 94% experience dissonance with respect to Enjoyment. In general, younger travelers tend to experience dissonance more frequently, with dissonance being less frequent for older travelers. The exception to this trend is Luxury, an affective value for which older travelers (age >54) experience dissonance more frequently than mid-aged travelers (ages 35-54).

Education

The results do not allow us to reject the hypothesis of equal levels of dissonance by levels of education for the values of Freedom, Enjoyment, Happiness, and Poverty. In contrast we can reject the null hypothesis in the case of two affective values, namely Luxury and Status. In the case of Luxury, dissonance is more frequent among people who have only K-12 education, and less frequently, albeit still high, for people with technical/university level education and post-graduate education. Furthermore, highly educated people (with postgraduate degrees) experience dissonance with respect to Status more frequently than with respect to Luxury.

Income

The next individual attribute that we examine is income, and in this case we detect significant differences in dissonance for three affective values: Poverty, Luxury, and Status. Poverty is a negative affect, and can be associated with the lack of a car (Reutter et al. 2009). In our case, we see that lower income individuals tend to associate this feeling to their primary mode of commuting with higher frequency (almost 20% of the time) than other income groups. Whereas approximately 16% of mid-income people are dissatisfied with their primary mode of travel when it comes to evoking feelings of Poverty, less than 7% of high income individuals are. Dissonance with respect to Luxury and Status also tends to be more common among lower income individuals, and declines substantially for mid- and high income respondents. Notice as well that the frequency of dissonance is higher in terms of Luxury than Status for mid- and high income people.

Primary mode of travel

The variable that shows the largest differences in the frequency of dissonance is the primary mode of travel. It can be seen in Table that the differences are significant for all six affective values. Compared to users of other modes of transportation, dissonance is particularly acute for users of public transportation when it comes to the values of Freedom, Enjoyment, and Happiness: almost 100% of users of public transportation have identified other mode or modes as better representing those affective values. Dissonance on these values is the least for active travelers: less than 50% of respondents who are active travelers associate Freedom to a different mode, and only around 60% identified a different mode when responding to the values of Enjoyment and Happiness, compared to approximately 79% and 71% of those whose primary mode is car. The picture changes when the values of Poverty, Luxury, and Status are considered. In this case, dissonance is less frequent for people who travel by car: less than 3% of car users associate the car with feelings of Poverty, only 41% associate Luxury with a mode other than car, and only about 31% relate Status to a different mode. Dissonance is more frequent in these values for active travelers, and users of public transportation and other modes, in no case being lower than 75%. This figure is virtually as high as 100% for users of public transportation, a group of travelers who consistently associate Luxury and Status with modes other than public transportation.

Typical commute time

Turning now to typical commute time, four affective values show significant differences with (p\leq0.10): Freedom, Happiness, Luxury, and Status. Perhaps not surprisingly, dissonance is more frequent among people whose typical commutes are longer. This is in line with previous findings: both St-Louis et al. (2014) and Smith (2017) report that commute satisfaction tends to decline with longer commutes, and Handy and Thigpen (2019) found that commute distance was a negative covariate of commute satisfaction.

Percentage of respondents who report mode dissonance with respect to various affective values

Variable

Freedom

(\chi^2) p-val

Enjoyment

(\chi^2) p-val

Happiness

(\chi^2) p-val

Poverty

(\chi^2) p-val

Luxury

(\chi^2) p-val

Status

(\chi^2) p-val

Age

< 35

89.88

(<0.001)

94.33

(<0.001)

93.52

(<0.001)

87.63

0.4095

89.43

(<0.001)

87.17

(<0.001)

35 - 54

74.22

81.45

82.26

87.38

70.18

68.70

> 54

74.00

72.92

68.75

76.19

78.57

66.67

Education

K-12

85.98

0.9063

92.16

0.7005

89.22

0.9694

79.27

0.3608

95.10

0.0058

92.39

0.013

Tech/Univ

83.00

86.94

86.94

88.51

78.99

75.00

Graduate

78.57

85.19

84.62

86.36

76.00

79.17

Income

Low

86.51

0.5755

82.11

0.2264

88.62

0.9698

80.19

0.0642

88.71

0.0204

88.70

0.0137

Middle

84.52

89.80

88.00

83.76

85.82

80.00

High

79.19

90.97

85.82

93.69

73.68

70.31

Mode

Car

58.93

(< 0.001)

78.90

(<0.001)

70.91

(<0.001)

97.96

0.0045

41.00

(<0.001)

30.69

(<0.001)

Active

46.34

60.98

57.89

75.76

89.47

81.82

Public

99.23

96.76

98.80

81.35

100.00

99.57

Other

91.30

86.96

91.30

93.33

72.73

90.00

Commute Time

< 20 min

65.93

(<0.001)

82.95

0.6115

77.27

0.0394

86.11

0.9225

73.49

0.0925

67.09

0.0158

20 - 40 min

85.86

87.50

89.58

90.41

83.87

81.18

40 - 60 min

83.04

89.91

89.62

86.08

82.35

77.78

> 60 min

95.93

92.17

93.28

83.02

91.15

90.09

Which modes do travelers associate with affective values?

The preceding analysis indicates that there is significant mode dissonance along various dimensions and for various affective values. This is for the most part in line with previous research, although by examining different affective values individually instead of a summary measure of wellbeing, we are able to differentiate better the affective reactions of travelers. Less is known about the values that travelers associate with modes other than the one they use. For this reason, after developing a profile of the travelers who experience mode dissonance in the preceding section, we are now interested in the responses of travelers with respect to the modes they ideally tend to associate with various affective values. For this analysis we employ a visualization technique known as faceting. Bar charts are used to plot the proportions of respondents who associate each mode of transportation with an affective value. Faceting allows us to explore these proportions in a multivariate way by slicing the data according to additional attributes. The result is a visual representation of a multi-way contingency table. Please note that due to the small numbers of travelers who use modes categorized as “Other” we henceforth exclude them from the analysis.

We begin our investigation of the modes more frequently associated with different affective values by plotting the primary mode of travel and the modes associated with the values (see Figure ). We can think of the figure as a matrix of plots, with each dimension of the matrix a facet. In this figure, the columns of the matrix of plots correspond to different affective values, and the rows to different modes of primary mode of transportation, i.e., the mode actually used. The height of the bars in these plots is the proportion of respondents who selected mode (j = \text{Car, Active, Public}) as embodying the corresponding affective value.

We know from the preceding analysis (Table ) that users of public transportation tend to experience dissonance with significantly higher frequency than car users and active travelers. But which mode(s) would align better to their expectations with regards to the affective values under study? If we inspect the bottom row of plots, we see that public transport users most of the time associate Freedom, Enjoyment, and Happiness with active travel. Users of public transportation are more or less equally split in terms of associating Poverty with active travel and public transportation, whereas they overwhelmingly associate Luxury and Status with the private car.

Active travelers rarely experience dissonance with respect to Freedom, Enjoyment, and Happiness. And although the experience of dissonance is more frequent with respect to Luxury and Status, there is still a sizable plurality of active travelers who associate these two affective values with active travel, not the car (contrast these to the responses of public transit users.)

Interestingly, dissonance among car users with respect to Freedom, Enjoyment, and Happiness is relatively high. While car is still the most frequently chosen mode associated with Freedom among car users, the proportion of car users who identify Freedom with active travel is almost as high. And when it comes to Enjoyment and Happiness, car users experience significant dissonance and in fact a majority tend to indicate active travel as representing these two affective values. In contrast, car users display little dissonance with respect to feelings of Luxury and Status.

It is interesting to note that feelings of “Poverty” are associated with public transportation and active travel in almost exactly the same proportions, irrespective of the primary mode of travel.

Next, we further explore these responses after stratifying by age, education, income, and typical commute time. We test the underlying 3-way tables by means of the Cochran-Mantel-Haenszel (\chi^2) test of independence, (the (p)-values of the tests are reported in the figures.) The next set of figures adds a facet to the columns, so that each affective value is now further divided into columns that correspond to the levels of an individual attribute (i.e., age, education, income, and typical commute time.)

Plots for affective values; in the y-axis are the number of cases by primary mode of transportation, and in the x-axis are the modes selected for each affective value

Age

We find some interesting differences when exploring dissonance from the perspective of age of the travelers (see Figure ). For example, as seen in the preceding section, active travel is commonly associated with Freedom, Happiness, and Enjoyment, even by car users, but especially by users of public transportation. However, when we break this down by age, we notice that this tendency weakens as people age, and older travelers increasingly assign these affective values to the car. Furthermore, the tendency to associate Status with the car tends to increase with age. In contrast, the car loses in the value of Luxury with age among car and public transport users, but gains among active travelers after dropping among mid-aged respondents. Older travelers almost universally associate Status with the car.

Plots for affective values by age; in the y-axis is the proportion in the interval [0, 1], and the x-axis is the mode selected for each value (p-values are for Cochran-Mantel-Haenszel Chi-Squared Test)

Education

Significant differences in dissonance by education are detected for five affective values. As seen in Figure , the association between active travel and Freedom is weaker among car travelers who are less educated (i.e., education K-12). Feelings of enjoyment are more frequently associated with active travel, and more so for less educated (K-12) and highly educated (graduate) car users and active travelers. This is somewhat different among users of public transportation, a majority of whom still assign feelings of Freedom to active travel, but with a higher proportion of travelers who associate this affective value to the car, especially among highly educated individuals. Less educated travelers who use the car are less likely to associate the value of Happiness to active travel, and this changes among people with higher levels of academic achievement, for whom active travel more frequently evokes feelings of Happiness. As before, most respondents identify the car with the values of Luxury and Status, and in general, people with lower education declare positive associations with the car more frequently. The exception is Status among active travelers: the more highly educated the traveler, the lower the probability of dissonance with respect to this affective value.

Plots for affective values by level of education; in the y-axis is the proportion in the interval [0, 1], and the x-axis is the mode selected for each value (p-values are for Cochran-Mantel-Haenszel Chi-Squared Test)

Income

Like with education, there are significant differences in dissonance by income for five affective values. As seen in Figure , car users with lower incomes tend to associate Freedom and Enjoyment with active travel, but more with respect to Enjoyment. In contrast, Enjoyment is more frequently associated with active travel among more affluent car users. Active travelers seldom experience dissonance with respect to Freedom, Enjoyment, and Happiness, irrespective of income level. However, although public transport users still tend to link active travel with feelings of Freedom, Enjoyment, and Happiness, they remain more likely to attribute these values to the car.

Interestingly, despite lower income individuals experiencing dissonance more frequently with respect to feelings of Poverty, we do not find that those feelings are specific to a transportation mode. In contrast, we see again that Luxury and Status generally are almost always attributed to the car, but with some interesting differences by income and primary mode of travel. Indeed, higher income active travelers, although still more likely to associate Luxury and Status with the car, are more likely than users of other modes to associate these affects to active travel, perhaps due to an awareness of the benefits of walking and using the bicycle. Furthermore, high-income users of cars are more likely to experience consonance with respect to status and luxury, in what might be a form of self-congratulatory confirmation of success.

Plots for affective values by level of income; in the y-axis is the proportion in the interval [0, 1], and the x-axis is the mode selected for each value (p-values are for Cochran-Mantel-Haenszel Chi-Squared Test)

Typical commute time

The last dimension of dissonance that we examine is typical commute time. Four affective values are significant along this dimension, namely Freedom, Enjoyment, Happiness, and Status. Figure shows the way travelers associate different affective values to modes of transportation by length of typical commute.

Among car users, Freedom is associated with the car in all but the longest commutes (>60 min), in which case active travel is more frequently identified as the mode that evokes feelings of Freedom. The associations of active travel with Enjoyment and Happiness are consistently high among car users, although car becomes more frequently associated with this value for longer commutes. As we have seen before, active travelers seldom experience dissonance with respect to Freedom, Enjoyment, and Happiness, and if anything the strength of this association increases with longer commutes. Public transportation users again are split in their preference towards active travel and car as the modes more likely to evoke feelings of Freedom, Enjoyment, and Happiness, and there are only relatively minor differences in these preferences by length of commute. The last affective value that displays significant differences in dissonance by length of typical commute is Status. The car tends to dominate as the mode more frequently associated with this value among car and public transportation users. Active travelers, on the other hand, associate status more strongly with short or very long commutes: only when commutes are shorter than 20 minutes or longer than 60 minutes do active travelers associate feelings of Status with the car.

Plots for affective values by commute time; in the y-axis is the proportion in the interval [0, 1], and the x-axis is the mode selected for each value (p-values are for Cochran-Mantel-Haenszel Chi-Squared Test)

Summary and Concluding Remarks

The topic of subjective wellbeing has attracted considerable attention in recent years due to its relationship with health. As the world tries to move from a culture dominated by a century-long love affair with the automobile, there is a pressing need to understand how travelers perceive different modes of transportation from the lens of subjective wellbeing. Insights in this regard could prove valuable to develop and implement plans and policies to attract and retain users to healthier, more environmentally friendly transportation options (Chatterjee et al. 2019). For this reason, understanding mode dissonance, the extent to which the experience of travelers differs from their aspirations, is a worthwhile topic for research.

This paper contributes to the literature in three ways.

First, the research contributes to an emerging literature on the topic of transportation and subjective wellbeing in the context of the Global South (Al-Ayyash and Abou-Zeid 2019; Bejarano, Ceballos, and Maya 2017; Shao and Liang 2019; Van, Choocharukul, and Fujii 2014; Zorrilla, Hodgson, and Jopson 2019); to the best of our knowledge, the case of Chile has not yet been reported. Second, although there is an extensive literature on the enjoyment of commute and other affective values (see for instance Paez and Whalen 2010; Redmond and Mokhtarian 2001; Whalen, Páez, and Carrasco 2013; Ye and Titheridge 2017), from a hedonic and even eudaimonic perspectives the analysis has yet to be applied more fully in terms of distributional issues – i.e. which groups more commonly experience dissonance (see De Vos 2018). Third, the analysis shows the attitudes of people towards their primary mode and their perception towards ‘ideal modes’, complementing the studies based on affective factors on transit or active modes (Bornioli, Parkhurst, and Morgan 2019; De Vos and Witlox 2017), satisfaction (J. De Vos et al. 2019), or quality (Redman et al. 2013).

The premise of this study is that a key component of subjective wellbeing is the affective reaction to experience (see the definition of wellbeing offered by the OECD; 2013). In this paper we investigated mode dissonance from the perspective of six affective values. The research presented here was based on a sample of travelers in Santiago, the capital of Chile. Participants in this research were asked about their typical mode of travel, and then about the mode or modes that they associate with each of the six affective values. Analysis using hypothesis testing (tests of independence) and visualization techniques (bar charts with faceting) uncovered interesting patterns. Some of our findings are well aligned with previous research; for example, active travelers experience less dissonance than car users, and users of public transportation experience the most dissonance of all. However, by considering affective values separately instead of aggregating them into a single indicator of subjective wellbeing, we manage to preserve greater granularity with respect to various responses than most studies. This is important because hedonic/eudaimonic values are more frequently related to active travel (Freedom, Enjoyment, Happiness), while Poverty is more frequently related to public transportation and active travel. Luxury and Status, on the other hand, are more frequently associated to car.

Further delving into the question of which modes are associated with these affective values, we find that there are important differences in terms of the typical mode of travel. Active travelers experience dissonance with relatively little frequently with respect to Freedom, Enjoyment, and Happiness, but when they do, they tend to attach positive values to the car. Car users experience dissonance with respect to these affects more frequently than active travelers, and when they do, they strongly relate positive hedonic/eudaimonic values to active travel. In other words, it is possible that drivers dream of walking when it comes to feelings of Enjoyment and Happiness, and to a lesser extent Freedom. The other side of the coin is also interesting. When it comes to affective values with a stronger socio-economic flavor, such as Poverty, Luxury, and Status, car users tend to experience dissonance less frequently than users of other modes. Active travelers, although more resistant to the lure of the car compared to users of public transportation, also tend to attach values of Luxury and Status to the car when they experience dissonance.

An examination of these effects by age, level of education, level of income, and typical trip duration reveals some ways in which these trends become more pronounced. For instance, older people are less likely than younger people to associate active travel with positive hedonic/eudaimonic affects, and are more likely to attach these values to the car. People with higher incomes are more atuned to the luxury and status values of cars, whereas lower income people are more likely to relate active travel to luxury and status.

These results not only help to flesh out some ways in which mode dissonance could play out from the perspective of different affects, but does so in the context of a Latin American country, a region where deep-seated taboos with respect to different modes of transportation exist: the poor travel by public transportation and/or are forced active travelers; a country where the rich enjoy the luxury of private vehicles and/or are active travelers by choice. In this way, the paper helps us to reflect on the affective reaction of members of the public with respect to their transportation experience. A better understanding of these responses can in turn be used to judiciously enhance the experience of commuting, to increase the satisfaction of commuters, and to meet their preferences.

In this respect, it is important to note that the perception of users has been considered mainly from the lens of trip satisfaction, but the more granular approach taken in this study offers some advantages. For example, understanding dissonances reveals that public transport users are clearly at a disadvantage (i.e., they experience dissonance more frequently) compared to other travelers. Furthermore, the results reveal that within public transport users, there are also important socio-economic differences. In contrast, users of cars do not necessarily associate hedonic/eudaimonic values to this mode, which suggests that not only these feelings could be leveraged to attract them to active modes, but could also as a result lead to gains in wellbeing.

With respect to opportunities for future research, a possible extension of the present research would be to investigate the use of the modes other than the typical mode of transportation. In a recent paper that investigated commute satisfaction for car users, Al-Ayyash and Abou-Zeid (2019) considered three models: for current trip satisfaction, remembered satisfaction while using public transport, and current satisfaction using public transport. The findings suggest that low service quality in public transportation can result in a generalized negative perception, and that this perception is more difficult to smooth if commuters do not regularly use public transport. Another avenue for future research could be to consider the mix of modes typically used. While in this paper the analysis focused on the primary mode of transportation, many travelers experience more than one mode of transportation in their daily activities. For this reason, considering the multimodal component of travel would be interesting; for example, future research could consider people who eventually arrive by bicycle to the metro station or people that, after using a colectivo for part of a trip, end their journey by bus.

Acknowledgments

We wish to express our gratitude to Prof. Jonas De Vos and three anonymous reviewers for their feedback on an earlier version of this paper. Their suggestions helped to improve the quality of the research and the clarity of the presentation. The following R packages were used in the course of this investigation and the authors wish to acknowledge their developers: ggmosaic (Jeppson, Hofmann, and Cook 2018), ggthemes (Arnold 2018), kableExtra (Zhu 2018), knitr (Xie 2015, 2018), rticles (Allaire et al. 2018), and tidyverse (Wickham 2017).

References

Abenoza, Roberto F, Oded Cats, and Yusak O Susilo. 2017. “Travel Satisfaction with Public Transport: Determinants, User Classes, Regional Disparities and Their Evolution.” Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 95: 64–84.

Al-Ayyash, Z., and M. Abou-Zeid. 2019. “Investigating Commute Satisfaction Differences of Private Car Users and Public Transport Users in a Developing Country Context.” Journal Article. Transportation 46 (3): 515–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11116-019-10000-2.

Allaire, JJ, Yihui Xie, R Foundation, Hadley Wickham, Journal of Statistical Software, Ramnath Vaidyanathan, Association for Computing Machinery, et al. 2018. Rticles: Article Formats for R Markdown. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rticles.

Anable, J., and B. Gatersleben. 2005. “All Work and No Play? The Role of Instrumental and Affective Factors in Work and Leisure Journeys by Different Travel Modes.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part A 39 (2-3): 163–81. ISI:000227194400005 C:/Papers/Transportation Research A/Transportation Research A (2005) 39 (2-3) 163-181.pdf http://ac.els-cdn.com/S096585640400093X/1-s2.0-S096585640400093X-main.pdf?_tid=e469e7f2-044e-11e7-8d8a-00000aacb35d&acdnat=1489012434_0c0c06f69282041cb51842a427d42bac.

Angotti, Thomas. 1996. “Latin American Urbanization and Planning: Inequality and Unsustainability in North and South.” Journal Article. Latin American Perspectives 23 (4): 12–34. https://doi.org/10.1177/0094582X9602300403.

Argyle, Michael, D Kahneman, E Diener, and N Schwarz. 1999. “Causes and Correlates of Happiness Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology.(pp. 353-373).” New York, NY US: Russell Sage Foundation.

Arnold, Jeffrey B. 2018. Ggthemes: Extra Themes, Scales and Geoms for ’Ggplot2’. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ggthemes.

Avellaneda, Pau, and Alejandra Lazo. 2011. “APROXIMACION a La Movilidad Cotidiana En La Periferia Pobre de Dos Ciudades Latinoamericanas. LOS Casos de Lima Y Santiago de Chile.” Journal Article. Revista Transporte Y Territorio, no. 4: 47–58.

Bejarano, M., L. M. Ceballos, and J. Maya. 2017. “A User-Centred Assessment of a New Bicycle Sharing System in Medellin.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part F-Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 44: 145–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2016.11.004.

Bocarejo, S. J. P., and H. D. R. Oviedo. 2012. “Transport Accessibility and Social Inequities: A Tool for Identification of Mobility Needs and Evaluation of Transport Investments.” Journal Article. Journal of Transport Geography 24: 142–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2011.12.004.

Bornioli, A., G. Parkhurst, and P. L. Morgan. 2019. “Affective Experiences of Built Environments and the Promotion of Urban Walking.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part a-Policy and Practice 123: 200–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2018.12.006.

Boschmann, E. E., and M. P. Kwan. 2008. “Toward Socially Sustainable Urban Transportation: Progress and Potentials.” Journal Article. International Journal of Sustainable Transportation 2 (3): 138–57. ISI:000261392400002 C:/Papers/International Journal of Sustainable Transportation/International Journal of Sustainable Transportation (2008) 2 (3) 138-157.pdf.

Brown, J. R., E. A. Morris, and B. D. Taylor. 2009. “Planning for Cars in Cities: Planners, Engineers, and Freeways in the 20th Century.” Journal Article. Journal of the American Planning Association 75 (2): 161–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/01944360802640016.

Brunsdon, Chris, and Alexis Comber. 2020. “Opening Practice: Supporting Reproducibility and Critical Spatial Data Science.” Journal Article. Journal of Geographical Systems. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10109-020-00334-2.

Cao, X. Y., and D. F. Ettema. 2014. “Satisfaction with Travel and Residential Self-Selection: How Do Preferences Moderate the Impact of the Hiawatha Light Rail Transit Line?” Journal Article. Journal of Transport and Land Use 7 (3): 93–108. https://doi.org/10.5198/jtlu.v7i3.485.

Chapman, L. 2007. “Transport and Climate Change: A Review.” Journal Article. Journal of Transport Geography 15 (5): 354–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2006.11.008.

Chatterjee, K., S. Chng, B. Clark, A. Davis, J. De Vos, D. Ettema, S. Handy, A. Martin, and L. Reardon. 2019. “Commuting and Wellbeing: A Critical Overview of the Literature with Implications for Policy and Future Research.” Journal Article. Transport Reviews, 30. https://doi.org/10.1080/01441647.2019.1649317.

Clark, Andrew E, and Andrew J Oswald. 1996. “Satisfaction and Comparison Income.” Journal of Public Economics 61 (3): 359–81.

Cox, Tomás, and Ricardo Hurtubia. 2016. “Vectores de Expansión Urbana Y Su Interacción Con Los Patrones Socioeconómicos Existentes En La Ciudad de Santiago.” Journal Article. EURE (Santiago) 42 (127): 185–207.

Davies, Richard. 2019. “Why Is Inequality Booming in Chile? Blame the Chicago Boys.” The Guardian, November.

Delbosc, A. 2012. “The Role of Well-Being in Transport Policy.” Journal Article. Transport Policy 23: 25–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tranpol.2012.06.005.

De Vos, J. 2018. “Do People Travel with Their Preferred Travel Mode? Analysing the Extent of Travel Mode Dissonance and Its Effect on Travel Satisfaction.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part a-Policy and Practice 117: 261–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2018.08.034.

———. 2019. “Satisfaction-Induced Travel Behaviour.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part F-Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 63: 12–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2019.03.001.

De Vos, Jonas, Patricia L Mokhtarian, Tim Schwanen, Veronique Van Acker, and Frank Witlox. 2016. “Travel Mode Choice and Travel Satisfaction: Bridging the Gap Between Decision Utility and Experienced Utility.” Journal Article. Transportation 43 (5): 771–96.

De Vos, J., T. Schwanen, V. Van Acker, and F. Witlox. 2013. “Travel and Subjective Well-Being: A Focus on Findings, Methods and Future Research Needs.” Journal Article. Transport Reviews 33 (4): 421–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/01441647.2013.815665.

———. 2015. “How Satisfying Is the Scale for Travel Satisfaction?” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part F-Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 29: 121–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2015.01.007.

———. 2019. “Do Satisfying Walking and Cycling Trips Result in More Future Trips with Active Travel Modes? An Exploratory Study.” Journal Article. International Journal of Sustainable Transportation 13 (3): 180–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/15568318.2018.1456580.

De Vos, J., and F. Witlox. 2017. “Travel Satisfaction Revisited. On the Pivotal Role of Travel Satisfaction in Conceptualising a Travel Behaviour Process.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part a-Policy and Practice 106: 364–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2017.10.009.

Diener, ED, and M Eunkook Suh. 1997. “Subjective Well-Being and Age: An International Analysis.” Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics 17: 304–24.

Diener, E., S. Oishi, and R. E. Lucas. 2015. “National Accounts of Subjective Well-Being.” Journal Article. American Psychologist 70 (3): 234–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038899.

Dolan, P., and R. Metcalfe. 2012. “Measuring Subjective Wellbeing: Recommendations on Measures for Use by National Governments.” Journal Article. Journal of Social Policy 41: 409–27. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0047279411000833.

Domarchi, C., A. Tudela, and A. Gonzalez. 2008. “Effect of Attitudes, Habit and Affective Appraisal on Mode Choice: An Application to University Workers.” Journal Article. Transportation 35 (5): 585–99. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11116-008-9168-6.

Eriksson, Lars, Margareta Friman, and Tommy Gärling. 2013. “Perceived Attributes of Bus and Car Mediating Satisfaction with the Work Commute.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 47: 87–96.

Ettema, D., T. Garling, L. Eriksson, M. Friman, L. E. Olsson, and S. Fujii. 2011. “Satisfaction with Travel and Subjective Well-Being: Development and Test of a Measurement Tool.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part F-Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 14 (3): 167–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2010.11.002.

Ettema, D., T. Garling, L. E. Olsson, and M. Friman. 2010. “Out-of-Home Activities, Daily Travel, and Subjective Well-Being.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part a-Policy and Practice 44 (9): 723–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2010.07.005.

Feldman, Roberta M. 1990. “Settlement-Identity: Psychological Bonds with Home Places in a Mobile Society.” Environment and Behavior 22 (2): 183–229.

Ferrer-i-Carbonell, Ada. 2005. “Income and Well-Being: An Empirical Analysis of the Comparison Income Effect.” Journal of Public Economics 89 (5-6): 997–1019.

Friman, Margareta, Satoshi Fujii, Dick Ettema, Tommy Gärling, and Lars E Olsson. 2013. “Psychometric Analysis of the Satisfaction with Travel Scale.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 48: 132–45.

Garling, T., S. Bamberg, and M. Friman. 2019. THE Role of Attitude in Choice of Travel, Satisfaction with Travel, and Change to Sustainable Travel. Book. Handbook of Attitudes, Vol 2: Applications, 2nd Ed. <Go to ISI>://WOS:000461334100018.

Gatersleben, B., and D. Uzzell. 2007. “Affective Appraisals of the Daily Commute - Comparing Perceptions of Drivers, Cyclists, Walkers, and Users of Public Transport.” Journal Article. Environment and Behavior 39 (3): 416–31. ISI:000245893700007 C:/Papers/Environment and Behavior/Environment and Behavior (2007) 39 (3) 416-431.pdf.

Gossling, S., and S. Cohen. 2014. “Why Sustainable Transport Policies Will Fail: EU Climate Policy in the Light of Transport Taboos.” Journal Article. Journal of Transport Geography 39: 197–207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2014.07.010.

Handy, S., and C. Thigpen. 2019. “Commute Quality and Its Implications for Commute Satisfaction: Exploring the Role of Mode, Location, and Other Factors.” Journal Article. Travel Behaviour and Society 16: 241–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tbs.2018.03.001.

Jakobsson Bergstad, C., A. Gamble, T. Gärling, O. Hagman, M. Polk, D. Ettema, M. Friman, and L. E. Olsson. 2011. “Subjective Well-Being Related to Satisfaction with Daily Travel.” Journal Article. Transportation 38 (1): 1–15.

Jakobsson Bergstad, C., A. Gamble, O. Hagman, M. Polk, T. Garling, and L. E. Olsson. 2011. “Affective-Symbolic and Instrumental-Independence Psychological Motives Mediating Effects of Socio-Demographic Variables on Daily Car Use.” Journal Article. Journal of Transport Geography 19 (1): 33–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2009.11.006.

Jeppson, Haley, Heike Hofmann, and Di Cook. 2018. Ggmosaic: Mosaic Plots in the ’Ggplot2’ Framework. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ggmosaic.

Khreis, H., K. M. Warsow, E. Verlinghieri, A. Guzman, L. Pellecuer, A. Ferreira, I. Jones, et al. 2016. “The Health Impacts of Traffic-Related Exposures in Urban Areas: Understanding Real Effects, Underlying Driving Forces and Co-Producing Future Directions.” Journal Article. Journal of Transport & Health 3 (3): 249–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2016.07.002.

Lavery, T. A., A. Paez, and P. S. Kanaroglou. 2013. “Driving Out of Choices: An Investigation of Transport Modality in a University Sample.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part A-Policy and Practice 57: 37–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2013.09.010.

Lois, D., and M. Lopez-Saez. 2009. “The Relationship Between Instrumental, Symbolic and Affective Factors as Predictors of Car Use: A Structural Equation Modeling Approach.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part a-Policy and Practice 43 (9-10): 790–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2009.07.008.

Lucas, K. 2012. “Transport and Social Exclusion: Where Are We Now?” Journal Article. Transport Policy 20: 107–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tranpol.2012.01.013.

Lucas, Karen. 2019. “A New Evolution for Transport-Related Social Exclusion Research?” Journal of Transport Geography, 102529. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2019.102529.

Martens, Karel, Aaron Golub, and Glenn Robinson. 2012. “A Justice-Theoretic Approach to the Distribution of Transportation Benefits: Implications for Transportation Planning Practice in the United States.” Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 46 (4): 684–95.

Martínez, Cristhian Figueroa, Frances Hodgson, Caroline Mullen, and Paul Timms. 2018. “Creating Inequality in Accessibility: The Relationships Between Public Transport and Social Housing Policy in Deprived Areas of Santiago de Chile.” Journal Article. Journal of Transport Geography 67: 102–9.

Miller, Harvey J. 2011. “Collaborative Mobility: Using Geographic Information Science to Cultivate Cooperative Transportation Systems.” Journal Article. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 21 (0): 24–28. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.07.005.

Milne, Eugene M. G. 2012. “A Public Health Perspective on Transport Policy Priorities.” Journal Article. Journal of Transport Geography 21: 62–69. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2012.01.013.

Mokhtarian, P. L., and R. M. Pendyala. 2018. “Travel Satisfaction and Well-Being.” Book Section. In Quality of Life and Daily Travel, edited by M. Friman, D. Ettema, and L. E. Olsson, 17–39. Applying Quality of Life Research-Best Practices. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-76623-2_2.

Niehaus, Markus, Patricia Galilea, and Ricardo Hurtubia. 2016. “Accessibility and Equity: An Approach for Wider Transport Project Assessment in Chile.” Journal Article. Research in Transportation Economics 59: 412–22.

OECD. 2013. OECD Guidelines on Measuring Subjective Well-Being. OECD Publishing.

Paez, A., and K. Whalen. 2010. “Enjoyment of Commute: A Comparison of Different Transportation Modes.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part a-Policy and Practice 44 (7): 537–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2010.04.003.

Pereira, Rafael HM, Tim Schwanen, and David Banister. 2017. “Distributive Justice and Equity in Transportation.” Transport Reviews 37 (2): 170–91.

Redman, Lauren, Margareta Friman, Tommy Gärling, and Terry Hartig. 2013. “Quality Attributes of Public Transport That Attract Car Users: A Research Review.” Journal Article. Transport Policy 25: 119–27.

Redmond, L. S., and P. L. Mokhtarian. 2001. “The Positive Utility of the Commute: Modeling Ideal Commute Time and Relative Desired Commute Amount.” Journal Article. Transportation 28 (2): 179–205. ISI:000167854000005 C:/Papers/Transportation/Transportation (2001) 28 (2) 179-205.pdf.

Reutter, Linda I., Miriam J. Stewart, Gerry Veenstra, Rhonda Love, Dennis Raphael, and Edward Makwarimba. 2009. “‘Who Do They Think We Are, Anyway?’: Perceptions of and Responses to Poverty Stigma.” Journal Article. Qualitative Health Research 19 (3): 297–311. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732308330246.

Rodríguez Vignoli, Jorge. 2008. “Movilidad Cotidiana, Desigualdad Social Y Segregación Residencial En Cuatro Metrópolis de América Latina.” Journal Article. EURE (Santiago) 34 (103): 49–71.

Rojas, C., A. Paez, O. Barbosa, and J. Carrasco. 2016. “Accessibility to Urban Green Spaces in Chilean Cities Using Adaptive Thresholds.” Journal Article. Journal of Transport Geography 57: 227–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2016.10.012.

Schwanen, T., and P. L. Mokhtarian. 2004. “The Extent and Determinants of Dissonance Between Actual and Preferred Residential Neighborhood Type.” Journal Article. Environment and Planning B-Planning & Design 31 (5): 759–84. ISI:000224056800008 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3547633/pdf/nihms429217.pdf.

Shao, P., and J. Liang. 2019. “An Analysis of the Factors Influencing the Sustainable Use Intention of Urban Shared Bicycles in China.” Journal Article. Sustainability 11 (10). https://doi.org/10.3390/su11102721.

Smith, O. 2017. “Commute Well-Being Differences by Mode: Evidence from Portland, Oregon, Usa.” Journal Article. Journal of Transport & Health 4: 246–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2016.08.005.

Steg, L. 2005. “Car Use: Lust and Must. Instrumental, Symbolic and Affective Motives for Car Use.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part A 39 (2-3): 147–62.

St-Louis, E., K. Manaugh, D. van Lierop, and A. El-Geneidy. 2014. “The Happy Commuter: A Comparison of Commuter Satisfaction Across Modes.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part F-Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 26: 160–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2014.07.004.

Susilo, Y. O., and O. Cats. 2014. “Exploring Key Determinants of Travel Satisfaction for Multi-Modal Trips by Different Traveler Groups.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part a-Policy and Practice 67: 366–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2014.08.002.

Tesch-Römer, Clemens, Andreas Motel-Klingebiel, and Martin J Tomasik. 2008. “Gender Differences in Subjective Well-Being: Comparing Societies with Respect to Gender Equality.” Social Indicators Research 85 (2): 329–49.

Tiznado-Aitken, Ignacio, Juan Carlos Muñoz, and Ricardo Hurtubia. 2016. “How Equitable Is Access to Opportunities and Basic Services Considering the Impact of the Level of Service? The Case of Santiago, Chile.” Conference Proceedings. In. International Transport Forum Discussion Paper.

Van, H. T., K. Choocharukul, and S. Fujii. 2014. “The Effect of Attitudes Toward Cars and Public Transportation on Behavioral Intention in Commuting Mode Choice-a Comparison Across Six Asian Countries.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part a-Policy and Practice 69: 36–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2014.08.008.

Whalen, Kate E., Antonio Páez, and Juan A. Carrasco. 2013. “Mode Choice of University Students Commuting to School and the Role of Active Travel.” Journal Article. Journal of Transport Geography 31: 132–42. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2013.06.008.

Wickham, Hadley. 2017. Tidyverse: Easily Install and Load the ’Tidyverse’. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=tidyverse.

Xie, Yihui. 2015. Dynamic Documents with R and Knitr. 2nd ed. Boca Raton, Florida: Chapman; Hall/CRC. https://yihui.name/knitr/.

———. 2018. Knitr: A General-Purpose Package for Dynamic Report Generation in R. https://yihui.name/knitr/.

Ye, R. N., and H. Titheridge. 2017. “Satisfaction with the Commute: The Role of Travel Mode Choice, Built Environment and Attitudes.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part D-Transport and Environment 52: 535–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trd.2016.06.011.

Zhu, Hao. 2018. KableExtra: Construct Complex Table with ’Kable’ and Pipe Syntax. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=kableExtra.

Zorrilla, M. C., F. Hodgson, and A. Jopson. 2019. “Exploring the Influence of Attitudes, Social Comparison and Image and Prestige Among Non-Cyclists to Predict Intention to Cycle in Mexico City.” Journal Article. Transportation Research Part F-Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 60: 327–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2018.10.009.

About

Mella Lira, B., Paez, A. (2021) Do drivers dream of walking? An investigation of travel mode dissonance from the perspective of affective values, Journal of Transport and Health, 20:101015

Topics

Resources

Stars

Watchers

Forks

Releases

No releases published

Packages

No packages published