An acquired taste is an appreciation for something unlikely to be enjoyed by a person who has not had substantial exposure to it. It is the opposite of innate taste, which is the appreciation for things that are enjoyable by most people without prior exposure to them.

Characteristics

edit

In case of food and drink, the difficulty of enjoying the product may be due to a strong or unpleasant odor, taste, mouthfeel (such as sashimi and sushi featuring uncooked seafood), appearance, or association (such as eating insects or organ meat).

Acquisition

edit

General

edit

The process of acquiring a taste can involve developmental maturation, genetics (of both taste sensitivity and personality), family example, and biochemical reward properties of foods. Infants are born preferring sweet foods and rejecting sour and bitter tastes, and they develop a preference for salt at approximately 4 months. However, vegetables tend to be a favourite as they start to learn to feed themselves. Neophobia (fear of novelty) tends to vary with age in predictable, but not linear, ways. Babies just beginning to eat solid foods generally accept a wide variety of foods, toddlers and young children are relatively neophobic towards food, and older children, adults, and the elderly are often adventurous eaters with wide-ranging tastes.[1]

The general personality trait of novelty-seeking does not necessarily correlate highly with willingness to try new foods. Level of food adventurousness may explain much of the variability of food preferences observed in "supertasters". Supertasters are highly sensitive to bitter, spicy, and pungent flavours, and some avoid them and like to eat only mild, plain foods, but many supertasters who have high food adventurousness enjoy these intense flavors and seek them out.[2] Some chemicals or combinations of chemicals in foods provide both flavor and beneficial or enjoyable effects on the body and mind and may be reinforcing, leading to an acquired taste. A study that investigated the effect of adding caffeine and theobromine (active compounds in chocolate) vs. a placebo to identically flavored drinks that participants tasted several times, yielded the development of a strong preference for the drink with the compounds.[3]

Intentional

edit

Intentionally changing one's preferences can be hard to accomplish. It usually requires a deliberate effort, acting as if one likes something in order to have the responses and feelings that will produce the desired taste. The challenge becomes one of distinguishing authentic or legitimate acquired tastes as a result of deeply considered preference changes from inauthentic ones motivated by status or conformity.[4][5]

Examples

edit

The following items can be described as "acquired tastes", often due to combination of both unfamiliarity and intensity of taste. In principle, though, anything for which one can have a taste, can also become an acquired taste. An acquired taste is distinguished by how one comes to have the taste, not what the thing in question is.

See also

edit

References

edit
  1. ^ Birch, L. L. (1999). "Development of food preferences". Annual Review of Nutrition. 19 (1): 41–62. doi:10.1146/annurev.nutr.19.1.41. PMID 10448516.41-62&rft.date=1999&rft_id=info:doi/10.1146/annurev.nutr.19.1.41&rft_id=info:pmid/10448516&rft.aulast=Birch&rft.aufirst=L. L.&rfr_id=info:sid/en.wikipedia.org:Acquired taste" class="Z3988">
  2. ^ Ullrich, N. V.; Touger-Decker, R.; O’Sullivan-Maillet, J.; Tepper, B. J. (2004). "PROP taster status and self-perceived food adventurousness influence food preferences". Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 104 (4): 543–549. doi:10.1016/j.jada.2004.01.011. PMID 15054337.543-549&rft.date=2004&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.jada.2004.01.011&rft_id=info:pmid/15054337&rft.aulast=Ullrich&rft.aufirst=N. V.&rft.au=Touger-Decker, R.&rft.au=O’Sullivan-Maillet, J.&rft.au=Tepper, B. J.&rfr_id=info:sid/en.wikipedia.org:Acquired taste" class="Z3988">
  3. ^ Smit, H. J.; Blackburn, R. J. (2005). "Reinforcing effects of caffeine and theobromine as found in chocolate". Psychopharmacology. 181 (1): 101–106. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-2209-3. PMID 15772863. S2CID 28790932.101-106&rft.date=2005&rft_id=https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:28790932#id-name=S2CID&rft_id=info:pmid/15772863&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s00213-005-2209-3&rft.aulast=Smit&rft.aufirst=H. J.&rft.au=Blackburn, R. J.&rfr_id=info:sid/en.wikipedia.org:Acquired taste" class="Z3988">
  4. ^ Bovens, L. (1992). "Sour Grapes and Character Planning". The Journal of Philosophy. 89 (2): 57–78. doi:10.2307/2027152. JSTOR 2027152.57-78&rft.date=1992&rft_id=info:doi/10.2307/2027152&rft_id=https://www.jstor.org/stable/2027152#id-name=JSTOR&rft.aulast=Bovens&rft.aufirst=L.&rfr_id=info:sid/en.wikipedia.org:Acquired taste" class="Z3988">
  5. ^ Bovens, L. (1995). "The Intentional Acquisition of Mental States". Philosophy and Phenomenological Research. 55 (4): 821–840. doi:10.2307/2108334. JSTOR 2108334.821-840&rft.date=1995&rft_id=info:doi/10.2307/2108334&rft_id=https://www.jstor.org/stable/2108334#id-name=JSTOR&rft.aulast=Bovens&rft.aufirst=L.&rfr_id=info:sid/en.wikipedia.org:Acquired taste" class="Z3988">
  6. ^ Absinthe, What is Absinthe? About its Science, Chemistry and Structure
  7. ^ Absinthe, the Potent Green Fairy

Further reading

edit
  • Otis, L. P. (1984). "Factors Influencing the Willingness to Taste Unusual Foods". Psychological Reports. 54 (3): 739–745. doi:10.2466/pr0.1984.54.3.739. S2CID 144931730.739-745&rft.date=1984&rft_id=info:doi/10.2466/pr0.1984.54.3.739&rft_id=https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:144931730#id-name=S2CID&rft.aulast=Otis&rft.aufirst=L. P.&rfr_id=info:sid/en.wikipedia.org:Acquired taste" class="Z3988">